Cerebral and Spinal Modulation of Pain by Emotions and Attention



Fig. 3.1
Model of emotions’ and attention’s effects on nociception, pain sensation, and pain’s primary and secondary affect. Neural structures likely to have a role in these dimensions are shown by abbreviations in adjacent parentheses. PC parietal cortex, S1–S2 primary and secondary somatosensory cortices, pINS posterior insula, ACC anterior cingulate cortex, aMCC anterior midcingulate cortex, aINS anterior insula, amy amygdala, PFC prefrontal cortex, Nacc nucleus accumbens, PAG periaqueductal gray, hyp hypothalamus, LPFC lateral prefrontal cortex, MPFC medial prefrontal cortex



Although the supraspinal mechanisms engaged by attention and emotions may substantially differ, their effects on pain-evoked activations appear to be difficult to disentangle due to the highly integrated nature of the pain-processing system. It has been suggested that one potential point of separation could be that attention preferentially affects the cerebral structures underlying the sensory dimension of pain, but recent neuroimaging findings are not perfectly congruent with this hypothesis (Villemure and Bushnell 2009; Ploner et al. 2011). It is also important to note that because of the partially sequential relationship between pain sensation and affect (Price 2000), modulation of pain sensation should also indirectly impact the affective dimension of pain, thereby blurring the boundaries between attentional and emotional effects. Another potential point of separation between attentional and emotional effects could be that emotions preferentially modulate pain affect, either directly or indirectly through a modulation of pain-related appraisals. However, distraction away from pain could also impact the affective dimension by interfering with the appraisal processes underlying pain’s primary and secondary affect, which would be consistent with recent reports of selective effects of attention on aINS activity (Ploner et al. 2011). Finally, emotions could also influence pain perception through modulation of autonomic activity, which could be misattributed to pain (Schachter and Wheeler 1962), although this possibility has yet to be formally tested.

Moreover, emotional states can also alter the direction of attention (Salovey 1992) and be associated with different spinal and supraspinal effects that can either work synergistically or antagonistically. Therefore, in order to identify the origins of emotional effects on pain-related brain activity, it is necessary to try to probe as much as possible the various levels of pain processing by combining several methodologies, including NFR recordings, measures of autonomic activity, fMRI, EEG, etc. One interesting avenue for future imaging studies could be the use of cross-validated multivariate pain “signatures” in order to further characterize the nature of the modulatory effects of various interventions (Wager et al. 2013). Hopefully, a better understanding of the psychological factors that influence pain will lead to a better understanding of pain itself, including how it may become dysregulated in chronic pain syndromes.



References



al’Absi M, Petersen KL (2003) Blood pressure but not cortisol mediates stress effects on subsequent pain perception in healthy men and women. Pain 106:285–295CrossRefPubMed


Amanzio M, Benedetti F (1999) Neuropharmacological dissection of placebo analgesia: expectation-activated opioid systems versus conditioning-activated specific subsystems. J Neurosci 19:484–494PubMed


Andre J, Zeau B, Pohl M et al (2005) Involvement of cholecystokininergic systems in anxiety-induced hyperalgesia in male rats: behavioral and biochemical studies. J Neurosci 25:7896–7904CrossRefPubMed


Arnold MB (1960) Emotion and personality psychological aspects, vol 1. Columbia University Press, New York


Atlas LY, Bolger N, Lindquist M, Wager TD (2010) Brain mediators of predictive cue effects on perceived pain. J Neurosci 30:12964–12977CrossRefPubMedCentralPubMed


Bartolo M, Serrao M, Gamgebeli Z et al (2013) Modulation of the human nociceptive flexion reflex by pleasant and unpleasant odors. Pain 154:2054–2059CrossRefPubMed


Bathien N (1971) Human spinal reflexes and attention levels. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 30:32–37CrossRefPubMed


Benedetti F, Mayberg HS, Wager TD et al (2005) Neurobiological mechanisms of the placebo effect. J Neurosci 25:10390–10402CrossRefPubMed


Berna C, Leknes S, Holmes E et al (2010) Induction of depressed mood disrupts emotion regulation neurocircuitry and enhances pain unpleasantness. Biol Psychiatry 67:1083–1090CrossRefPubMed


Bruehl S, Burns JW, Chung OY, Chont M (2012) Naloxone inhibits not only stress-induced analgesia but also sympathetic activation and baroreceptor-reflex sensitivity. Psychosom Med 73:612–619CrossRef


Buhle JT, Silvers JA, Wager TD et al (2014) Cognitive reappraisal of emotion: a meta-analysis of human neuroimaging studies. Cereb Cortex 24(11):2981–2990CrossRefPubMed


Bushnell MC, Duncan G, Dubner R et al (1985) Attentional influences on noxious and innocuous cutaneous heat detection in humans and monkeys. J Neurophysiol 5:1103–1110


Bushnell MC, Ceko M, Low L (2013) Cognitive and emotional control of pain and its disruption in chronic pain. Nat Rev Neurosci 14:502–511CrossRefPubMed


Butler RK, Finn DP (2009) Stress-induced analgesia. Prog Neurobiol 88:184–202CrossRefPubMed


Corbetta M, Shulman GL (2011) Spatial neglect and attention networks. Annu Rev Neurosci 34:569–599CrossRefPubMedCentralPubMed


Cornélio AM, Nunes-de-Souza RL, Morgan MM (2012) Contribution of the rostral ventromedial medulla to post-anxiety induced hyperalgesia. Brain Res 1450:80–86CrossRefPubMed


Craig AD (2003) A new view of pain as a homeostatic emotion. Trends Neurosci 26:303–307CrossRefPubMed


Danziger N (2006) Bases neurologiques de l’ affect douloureux. Rev Neurol 162:395–399CrossRefPubMed


Descartes R (1649) Les passions de l’âme. Henry Le Gras, Paris


Dowman R (2001) Attentional set effects on spinal and supraspinal responses to pain. Psychophysiology 38:451–464CrossRefPubMed


Dum J, Herz A (1984) Endorphinergic modulation of neural reward systems indicated by behavioral changes. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 21:259–266CrossRefPubMed


Edwards L, Ring C, France CR et al (2007) Nociceptive flexion reflex thresholds and pain during rest and computer game play in patients with hypertension and individuals at risk for hypertension. Biol Psychol 76:72–82CrossRefPubMedCentralPubMed


Fechir M, Breimhorst M, Kritzmann S et al (2012) Naloxone inhibits not only stress-induced analgesia but also sympathetic activation and baroreceptor-reflex sensitivity. Eur J Pain 16:82–92CrossRefPubMed


Fields HL (1999) Pain: an unpleasant topic. Pain Suppl 6:S61–S69CrossRef


Fields HL (2004) State-dependent opioid control of pain. Nat Rev Neurosci 5:565–575CrossRefPubMed


Fields HL (2007) Understanding how opioids contribute to reward and analgesia. Reg Anesth Pain Med 32:242–246CrossRefPubMed


Fisher JP, Hassan DT, O’Connor N (1995) Minerva. Br Med J 310:70CrossRef


Flor H, Birbaumer N, Schulz R et al (2002) Pavlovian conditioning of opioid and nonopioid pain inhibitory mechanisms in humans. Eur J Pain 6:395–402CrossRefPubMed


Foo H, Mason P (2005) Sensory suppression during feeding. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 102:16865–16869CrossRefPubMedCentralPubMed

Oct 21, 2016 | Posted by in PAIN MEDICINE | Comments Off on Cerebral and Spinal Modulation of Pain by Emotions and Attention

Full access? Get Clinical Tree

Get Clinical Tree app for offline access