Epidemiology has been defined as “the study of distribution and determinants of disease frequency in human populations.” Epidemiologic studies may be categorized according to whether they focus on describing the distribution of disease in a population (descriptive studies) or on elucidating the determinants of disease (analytic studies) (41). Studies on headache epidemiology have primarily been descriptive, giving data on headache prevalence in men and women, in various age groups, among different races, and in different countries. Descriptive studies have also provided information on the personal and societal impact of headache in terms of economic costs and reduced quality of life. Recently, a number of analytic epidemiologic studies have been conducted with the explicit aim of studying causation by considering whether the risk of headache is different for those exposed and not exposed to some factor of interest.
In this chapter, we review some important results from descriptive studies on the prevalence and incidence of headache, and also review epidemiologic evidence relating to comorbid conditions and suspected risk factors. In addition, we focus on some analytic and design issues related to headache epidemiology, using specific studies from the epidemiologic literature to illustrate methodologic issues.
HEADACHE CASE DEFINITION
Irrespective of the design or the purpose of an epidemiologic study, it is necessary to define who has a certain diagnosis (is a case) and who does not. Case definition may be a particular problem in headache because headache diagnoses usually are made on the basis of subjective experiences without any objective signs or markers. Furthermore, there is some overlap in symptomatology between headache subtypes, and multiple headache types often coexist in the same individual. The most comprehensive and elaborate system for classifying headache disorders is the International Classification of Headache Disorders, 2nd edition (ICHD-2) (44). These provide specific criteria that are partly based on expert opinion and partly on systematic studies on reliability and validity. They represent an evolution of the criteria published by the International Headache Society (IHS) in 1988 (20). The introduction of these criteria provided a foundation for headache epidemiology that was lacking in earlier research and has made it possible to make meaningful comparisons between studies.
Although the introduction of standardized diagnostic criteria has helped to move forward population studies of headache disorders, the way in which these criteria are interpreted and applied (operationalized) has varied somewhat between studies. For example, many individuals have more than one headache type, most often both migraine and tension-type headache. The IHS classification requires that each headache occurring in the same individual receives a separate diagnosis and that secondary causes of headache are excluded. The issues of multiple headache types and secondary headaches have been handled in different ways in population studies. The gold-standard headache diagnosis is made by personal interview and examination by a neurologist using structured diagnostic criteria. However, this approach is expensive (71) and has been used in only a few population studies, usually with some modifications (5,6,23,28,52,83,106,113). Expert diagnosis has the advantage of assessing unlimited coexisting headaches types and diagnosing rare headache syndromes as well as secondary causes of headache (80,103, 104, 105,107). However, secondary causes of headache are uncommon in the general population (84) and multiple headache types are of less importance when the study aim is to identify only migraine sufferers. Screening instruments by lay interviewers have been shown to be accurate when the aim is to identify only the most common headache types (e.g., migraine and tension-type headache). For example, one method allowed the diagnosis of two headache types and had 85% sensitivity and 96% specificity for diagnosing migraine (111). A “recognition-based” questionnaire has also been used for mass screening of headache in adolescents. In this technique, descriptions of migraine and tension-type headache based on the IHS criteria were read to the pupils in connection with a clinical examination, and the pupils with recurrent headaches were then asked to indicate which headache most closely resembled their own headache (126).
The way in which headache is defined can influence study results. For example, higher headache prevalence is found in answer to a neutral question (“Do you have headache?”) compared to questions involving some specification of headache degree (“Do you suffer from headache?” or “Do you have severe headache”?) (93). This is important when a question about headache is used to screen out non-headaches sufferers before the more specific questions about the features of the various headache types are posed. It has been found that those who answered “no” to the screening question about headache may nevertheless suffer from migraine, and that migraine prevalence increases after including screen-negative respondents (52). Ambiguities in the term headache may also be important when the term is translated, because it may have different connotations in other languages. This, as well as cultural differences relating to reporting pain, may contribute to variation in headache prevalence in different regions.
Because headache diagnosis ultimately depends on self-report, the quality of recall is of crucial importance. It is likely that recall is biased toward the most recent and severe headaches (80). The effect of recall problems was illustrated in one study (10), in which 41% of individuals interviewed in middle life could not remember that they had reported aura at an earlier stage. Recall problems may also explain why the lifetime prevalence of migraine decreased with increasing age in the Copenhagen study (83).
Finally, the definition of the control group is of equal importance to the definition of the case group. Headache is experienced at least occasionally by the great majority of the population (83). Choosing a control group that is without headache is thus likely not the best strategy, because being truly headache free is relatively rare. Recall problems may be a problem when trying to define a control group with relatively little or no headache. One study showed that some persons who answered negatively to a direct question on whether they had headache had relatively frequent headache when asked to keep a headache diary (121). This indicates that in many studies, the control group, which is allegedly “without headache,” may nevertheless have some. Therefore, some degree of misclassification of the controls as well as the cases is likely in most studies.
VALIDATION OF DIAGNOSTIC METHOD
In most headache epidemiologic studies, it is desirable to validate the headache diagnostic algorithm. This is ideally done by selecting a random sample of screen-positive and screen-negative individuals for a gold-standard diagnosis by a neurologist. By comparing the study diagnosis to the gold-standard diagnosis, the sensitivity (percent of true cases correctly identified) and specificity (percent of true noncases correctly identified) can be calculated (38,56,74,81). The sensitivity and specificity of the headache diagnosis affects the calculated rates of prevalence or incidence of disease in descriptive studies (e.g., [39]). In analytic studies, the usual effect of diagnostic error is to make measured associations between disease and risk factors more conservative because of imperfect differentiation between diseased and nondiseased individuals (bias towards the null hypothesis). In the context of a large population study (as opposed to a clinical diagnosis), a certain degree of imprecision is tolerable as long as the diagnostic error is unbiased.
A validation study should be done even if the questionnaire has already been validated, because one method may not be valid in other regions or countries, or at another time. The validation interview should be done in close temporal proximity to the main study so that any variation is caused by method and not to a change in the headache condition itself. In different validation studies, the degree of correspondence between the main study and the validation study is usually given by the kappa value, which is the observed agreement adjusted for the agreement rate expected by chance. For migraine diagnosis, the kappa values between clinical interview-based and questionnaire-based diagnoses of migraine has varied considerably, from 0.22 to 0.77 in various studies (38) with generally lower kappa values for tension-type headache.
SOURCE POPULATION AND SAMPLING
The source population is the population from which study participants are drawn. This is often a country, region, or city, but may also be schools, universities, or companies. The degree to which the results can be extrapolated to the general population depends on whether cases are representative of all cases and whether controls are representative of all noncases. A representative population can be obtained by drawing a sufficiently large random sample from the source population. Sometimes, however, a stratified sampling strategy is used to ensure that the study population resembles the source population with regard to some important features such as age, gender, race, or socioeconomic status (109).
Headache is a disorder that often does not lead to physician consultation (53,58,82,118). If the population of interest is headache sufferers in general, one may not get a representative sample by studying those who consult physicians, particularly those who consult headache specialists, because these patients likely have more frequent and severe headaches than the general population of headache sufferers, and they may also differ in many other respects.
The participation rate, namely, the proportion of the sampled population that actively participates in the study, can affect the degree to which the study population is representative of the source population. If headache is the main object of the study, individuals suffering from headache may be more likely to participate than non-headache sufferers, and the headache prevalence may be overrated. Likewise, if certain age or socioeconomic groups have a higher nonparticipation rate than the average, this may distort the results. A high participation rate is therefore important and it is also desirable that an evaluation of the nonparticipants is performed to assess whether they are different from the participating population with regard to age, gender, and socioeconomic status. In some cases, demographic information is available for the nonparticipants, and this information can be used to determine whether the nonparticipants are similar to the participants. Otherwise, the participating population can be compared to external sources of demographic data (e.g., census data for the country or town in which the study takes place) to determine whether the participating population appears to be reasonably representative. If participation is found to vary substantially by demographic characteristics, prevalence rates can be adjusted to compensate for differential participation (112).
TYPES OF EPIDEMIOLOGIC STUDIES
The study designs dealt with under the heading of epidemiology include both experimental and nonexperimental (observational) studies. Nonexperimental analytic study designs are often called observational because the investigator only observes those who are exposed or nonexposed. This is in contrast to experiments, clinical trials, or community interventions where the investigator assigns exposure to one group but not another. In this section, we discuss how various nonexperimental study designs have been applied to the field of headache epidemiology and give examples of the type of information these different designs can yield and some problems inherent in each of them (86).
In a case series, headache is described and often related to some other factor in a group of patients. Many interesting features of headache have been studied in this way. For instance, migraine attacks have been related to meteorologic factors (22), seasonal variation in daylight duration in polar areas (90), the menstrual cycle (110), the natural course of HIV infection (27), and seizures in epileptic patients (116). In addition, several studies have described the prevalence and special features of migraine and headache in patients with lupus (31), Tourette syndrome (50), idiopathic intracranial hypertension (117), glaucoma (75), and among visually impaired persons (47). Because there is no control group, only hypotheses about causal factors can be formulated on the basis of such studies.
With correlational (or ecologic) studies, the headache prevalence in a defined population is related to some other factor. By this method, relatively little used in headache epidemiology, it was found that headache among schoolchildren was more prevalent in districts with high unemployment (17), that headache prevalences in various parts of Greece correlated with mean temperatures (66), and that migraine was more prevalent at high altitude than at sea level in Peru (5). A limitation of this method is that exposure cannot be linked to particular individuals, and it is not possible to adjust for possible confounding factors.
In cross-sectional surveys, the disease status of individuals in the population is assessed at the same time as exposures of interest, such as demographic factors, comorbid conditions, or other suspected risk factors. A prevalence study is a cross-sectional study conducted to determine the proportion of the population that has a disease. Because headache usually varies considerably through life, it is important that a time span for the headache is determined. Lifetime prevalence measures the lifetime occurrence of headache. Period prevalence measures the proportion of individuals who have had headache during a defined period. One-year prevalence is often used because it is considered reasonably reliable, and it defines the proportion of the population that has an active disease, therefore being relevant for assessing the burden of headache in society.
Prevalence studies are probably the most common study type used in headache epidemiology. As can be seen from Table 3-1, prevalences differ widely between studies. Differences between studies using the same diagnostic criteria could be caused by differences in the age and gender distribution of the source populations, differences in the way in which the diagnostic criteria were operationalized, participation rates, or underlying differences in headache incidence or prognosis in different populations (93).
In virtually all studies in adults there is a higher proportion of headache sufferers among women than among men (see Table 3-1). Migraine prevalence typically increases in childhood and youth, is relatively stable and high in the third to the fifth decades, after which there is a marked decline in both sexes. Tension-type headache appears to be less related to age than migraine (97). A relatively typical age distribution of the 1-year headache prevalence in adults from a large population-based study in Norway is shown in Figure 3-1.
Studies in the United States and England have found higher prevalences of migraine among Caucasians (18,108), followed by African Americans and Asian Americans (111). In Singapore, the prevalence among the Chinese was lower than among the non-Chinese population (43). In a meta-analytic study of many prevalence surveys, the relative contributions of racial, geographic, and methodologic factors to the great variations in headache and migraine prevalence have been assessed (93).
TABLE 3-1 Population-Based Studies on Prevalence of Headache in Various Parts of the World